The evolution of description

Copyright 2016 Graham Berrisford.

One of about 300 articles at http://avancier.website. Last updated 12/01/2021 10:51

 

This is a supplement to the chapter introducing description theory here https://lnkd.in/dQNhNbd

It explores the evolution of description.

Contents

Preface  1

The big bang and space-time continuum.. 2

Sensing and reacting to continuous change. 2

Sensing and reacting to discrete things. 3

Remembering perceived entities and events. 3

Creating and using mental models. 3

Recognising family resemblances (fuzzy types) 4

Counting things. 5

Abstracting logical mental models from physical ones. 6

Verbalising mental models. 7

Using words to define and communicate fuzzy types. 7

Notes on types  9

Beware that natural language is an imperfect communication mechanism.. 9

The strict types used in mathematics and computing. 10

Fuzzy logic and artificial intelligence. 10

Conclusions and remarks. 11

 

Preface

The aim here is not to present a new “a priori” view of the world.

The aim is to integrate existing theories into a coherent story.

The story of description is well-nigh magical, and not limited to humans.

A honey bee can dance to describe where a discrete pollen source can be found.

It can communicate using a code that not only other bees but also humans can decode.

And recent experiments show that though a honey bee has a brain the size of a grain of sand, it can count up to four.

The big bang and space-time continuum

Heinz von Foerster (1911 to 2002) was a thinker interested in the circularity of ideas.

He is reputed to have said “We live in the domain of descriptions that we invented.”

But we don’t live in a universe we invented.

Scientists believe it started with a big bang about 14,000 million years ago.

In the beginning, there was a lot of energy, and then a lot of disordered matter.

Gradually, the laws of physics and evolution created things so orderly they can seem to be designed.

Planets fell into orderly repeating orbits; tides ebbed and flowed on a daily basis.

So now some things in the universe behaved in an orderly fashion.

 

The earth was formed about 4,500 million years ago.

And life on earth began at least 3,500 million years ago, possibly more.

There were things on earth before life, but no description of them.

There were no memories and messages.

There was no conceptualisation or model of things in space and time.

There were no representations or symbolisations of what exists and what happens.

Description is a side effect of biological evolution.

Sensing and reacting to continuous change

Darwinist evolution in biology is not goal-directed with a fixed forward-looking goal; rather, species adapt themselves to an ever-changing environment.”

http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/scientific-progress/#ReaIns

 

Eventually, by consuming energy, some matter became organised into an organic life form.

An organism is an individual life form - or single-celled entity, a plant, an animal.

It is a system - meaning an organization of parts that cooperate in repeated processes to a common end.

 

Plants and then animals evolved to sense and react to variations in continuous variables

Both internal variables (hydration, salinity, PH) and external variables (heat, light, sound).

They adapted their behavior to changes in these variables - autonomously.

 

Autonomic adaptation to change

Senses of variables

<inherit and use>           <abstract from>

Animals             <sense and react to>   Matter and energy

Sensing and reacting to discrete things

“A biological approach to human knowledge naturally gives emphasis to the pragmatist view that theories [descriptions of reality] function as instruments of survival.”

http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/scientific-progress/#ReaIns

 

The universe is an ever-unfolding process.

Physicists describe it as four-dimensional space-time continuum.

The word “continuum” implies space and time can be subdivided without any limit to size or duration.

 

As animals who perceive, remember and describe phenomena, we divide the universe into discrete things.

We divide it where the form of things changes – as on solid-fluid phase boundaries.

And where the state of things changes - such from night to day.

 

This implies animals hold an internal model of the sense of a thing.

The model is a bio/electro/chemical pattern of some kind, inherited from a parent.

The animal can match new perceptions to it, and act according to inherited rules.

 

Autonomic reactions

Sensations

<recognize>           <represent>

Animals      < detect and react to>     Phenomena

Remembering perceived entities and events

Animals evolved to recall new things they perceived.

They can record a first perception in a first sense memory.

The encode a second perception in a second sense memory.

Then match the two memories, and act according to whether they recognise a recurrence of the first thing.

.

Intelligent reactions

Sensations and memories

<recognize>              <represent>

Animals       <detect and react to>    Phenomena

 

This research suggests most animals remember particular entities better than particular events.

However, animals can recognise a pattern or sequence in which events happen.

This other research suggests even rats can replay memories in order to recognise things in sequence.

You can remember the sequence of steps in a dance, notes in a melody, or words in a story.

And of course, the sequence of words in a sentence or message is important to its meaning.

 

My notes on how the brain works suggests we know very little about how it works.

But how brains create and use descriptions of reality doesn’t matter here; it only matters that they evidently do.

 

Today, EA is about business activity systems that remember information,

The record and use information about entities and events they need to monitor and direct.

They use that memory to decide how to responds to events when they happen.

Creating and using mental models

Conant’s “Good Regulator Theorem” is a fundamental principle of cybernetics.

It says "every good regulator of a system must be a model of that system".

 

The good regulator

Models

<create and use>           <represent>

Regulators    <detect and direct >    Phenomena

 

Similarly, we humans monitor and direct entities and events in our environment.

We must have a model of those entities and events, however sketchy that model may be.

The existence of mental models is an abstraction for which we have only indirect evidence.

But that evidence is overwhelming (honey bees tell other bees where the pollen is, and they find it).

So, we can use the term without needing to explain how a mental model is made or remembered.

 

It is often said that perception is reality.

However, animals make predictions based on perceptions.

When predictions are tested and found to come true, then we have some assurance that, also, reality is perception.

That is how biological evolution helped animal with accurate sense models to survive, thrive and reproduce.

(This may be considered contrary to Wittgenstein’s argument that a private language is impossible).

Recognising family resemblances (fuzzy types)

To exclaim Danger! Is to convey a meaning, a concept, a type that classifies the current situation.

 

“In describing a situation, one is not merely registering a [perception], one is classifying it in some way, and this means going beyond what is immediately given.”

Chapter 5 of “Language truth and logic” A J Ayer.

 

To compare every new thing we see against every remembered thing would be a very inefficient way to look at the world.

If we can remember a common pattern of sense memories, then we can compare a new thing against that pattern.

This must be a more efficient way of recognising what a thing is and how to deal with it.

Evidently, animals have evolved to perceive the world as composed of things that share family resemblances.

And learning depends on the ability to detect resemblances things, and act accordingly.

 

Experiments show animals remember family resemblances – perhaps as a group of sense memories.

This forms an abstraction - a generalisation – a pattern - a “type” – that matches a set of similar things.

Typification

Types

<create and use>      <encode qualities of>

Describers         <observe>    Similar things

 

For example

Consider the shadow of a man on a cave wall; a pattern of light and shade.

It has no meaning until viewed by an actor who is able to recognise it as a description of something.

It might be recognised as the body shape of the man type.

 

Dogs use their sense memories to help them survive by recognising things and anticipating the behaviour of things.

A dog senses what it observes to be a discrete thing in reality, say a bone.

The dog has no words for the qualities and quantities that its senses detect.

Nevertheless, the dog does manage to encode a memory of the bone thing in its brain.

The memory might be a grouping of sensory measures (smell, taste, texture…) and experiences (chewable, pleasurable).

 

Later, when the dog finds a new thing that resembles the old thing, it can act according to its recorded experience.

The resemblance must - near enough - group same qualities in similar quantities.

Repeated observations of resemblances can strengthen the dog’s memory of how best to deal with things of that type.

The goal of this learning process is to optimize performance and minimize the number of mistakes made in dealing with new things.

 

Where do numbers come from?

If an animal can remember a general pattern or type, then a startling new idea emerges.

It becomes possible to count things of that type, and communicate the total number to others.

 

Abstraction of quantities from sets

Quantities

<create and use>      <represent totals of>

Counters    <observe and envisage>  Things of a type

 

The argument here is that logic and mathematics cannot exist until intelligences have crystallized family resemblances into types.

You can’t say a statement is true or false, until you know the types referred to.

·       You can’t say “This man is my brother” is true or false, until you know the properties of the brother type.

·       You can’t count your siblings until you know the properties of the sibling type.

·       You can’t count any things until you know the type to which they

 

Primitive animals surely don’t classify things into rigid “types”.

But they can recognize “family resemblances” between similar things (e.g. food items, friends and cliff edges).

And learn to respond to similar things in appropriate ways.

 

Scientists have studied how far honey bees, dolphins and babies can understand quantities.

We know many animals can recognize when a smallish family of similar things gains or loses a member

Experiments show dolphins can recognize which of two boards has, say, five dots rather than six.

And babies (before they have words) can recognize when a small group of things gains or loses a member.

 

Astonishingly, experiments suggest honey bees can count up to four and communicate that amount to other bees.

Honeybees are clever little creatures. They can form abstract concepts, such as symmetry versus asymmetry.

And they use symbolic language — the celebrated waggle dance — to direct their hivemates to flower patches.

New reports suggest that they can also communicate across species, and can count — up to a point.”

http://www.livescience.com/2909-bees-count.html

 

A number is a description made by a describer who can do three things.

1.     observe or envisage several discrete things

2.     either aggregate them (e.g. items in basket) or match them to a given type

3.     count the things in the aggregate or matching the type

 

Types enable counting; numbers imply types.

And types can be organised in type hierarchies.

Abstracting logical mental models from physical ones

Sense memories help animals to direct behavior to their advantage.

The direction can be autonomic: you sense heat and automatically starting sweating.

Or conscious: you see a train coming, predict it will run you over, and step off the track.

Animals who can form more accurate mental models survive and thrive better than those who form inaccurate models.

 

Idealisation triangle

Mental models

<create and use>   <abstract concepts from>

Animals         <observe and envisage>       Realities

 

Here, mental model does not mean a philosophy of life, world view or “Weltanschauung”.

It means a model of particular concepts/meanings that actors perceive or envisage in realities.

We form and store such mental models in our minds, in written words, and in other ways.

We share mental models using variety of communication mechanisms.

·       Gesturing animatedly to an oncoming train is enough to share the mental model of that as a threat to survival.

·       Spoken words translate mental models into transient sound waves – heard by a currently present audience.

·       Written words transcribe mental models into persistent graphical symbols – readable by future and remote audiences.

 

Logical and physical mental models.

Physical mental models are bio-electro-chemical

Logical mental models are concepts/meanings that actors perceive or envisage.

 

Physical mental models are private, not directly shareable.

Logical mental models are shareable between animals.

 

How do we know both kinds of models exist?

We define a logical model, and predict actions to follow from sharing it.

We test that the logical model has been shared between animals

Successful tests imply the logical model has been stored in a physical form.

 

E.g. we express the logical model represented in a honey bee’s dance as the “distance” and “direction” of a pollen source.

Then test that other bees find the pollen in the location thus described.

This shows those bees have stored mental models long enough to complete actions as predicted.

The storage of each physical mental model must be bio-electro-chemical, but how it works is irrelevant.

 

Physical models may be fragile, unstable, and inconsistent.

Nevertheless, evolution first made them good enough to be storable and actable on.

Then equipped animals with tools to share the logical information/meaning in them.

Verbalising mental models

Animals evolved to communicate descriptions of things to their fellows.

They translate their mental models into messages that others - even in other species – can interpret.

·       One bottlenose dolphin can recognise another by its signature whistle.

·       Honey bees dance to describe pollen locations.

·       Dogs bark to tell us a stranger has arrived.

·       Domesticated dogs can communicate several meanings in barks, growls, howls and whimpers.

 

The remainder of the story is essentially a human one.

Verbal descriptions of perceptions and memories are the foundation of advanced reasoning and communication.

They help us to communicate about complex things, both observed things and envisaged things.

We expect listeners/readers to recognise speakers/writers complex meanings, and act accordingly.

Using words to label, define and communicate fuzzy types

We humans developed the ability to communicate descriptions of realities using spoken words.

Then found we could describe words using words, and convey these definitions to a message receiver.

We developed ways to preserve and share descriptions using written words.

We invented dictionaries, and conventions for defining words (by genus and difference).

 

Long ago, astronomers observed things in the sky that shared the property of being a light source, a “star”.

They noticed some “stars” shared a second common property, that of being “wandering”.

They invented the type name “planet” as a short-hand label for these two properties of a planet.

 

The spoken word is transient, and has a limited audience.

To illustrate verbal type names and definitions, this work has to use the written form.

These

are described using a

which is to imply the ideas/properties in this

Things

Type name

Type elaboration

Venus, Mars etc.

Planet

Wandering thing, Starry thing

 

The table above shows how human actors idealise real things, and encode those ideas in verbal descriptions.

We use type names as a short-hand to describe things that are observed or envisaged.

 

By defining words using words, our brains surely grow a vast network of type names, associated with each other and with other sense memories.

Notes on types

A type might be recognised:

·       in a private language known only to one individual

·       in a domain-specific language shared by a specialist group

·       as a universal concept (universally useful and logically consistent with previously agreed universal types).

 

In the long view, types turn into states.

·       Over a short time, you see objects you can classify under structural types (e.g. child and adult).

·       Over a long time, apparently static object types now appear as the transient states of processes.

This is why type hierarchies don’t work well as persistent database structures.

Beware that natural language is an imperfect communication mechanism

The English language used here is rich in words that have multiple meanings and words that share meanings.

Consider these words: type, concept, property, quality, feature and attribute. They are all used definitive descriptions.

It seems some of these words work better with certain verbs others. E.g.

·       A rose bush is a plant that instantiates the types “thorny”, “flowering” and “bushy”

·       A rose bush is a plant that embodies the concepts “thorny”, “flowering” and “bushy”

·       A rose bush is a plant that exhibits the properties “thorny”, “flowering” and “bushy”

·       A rose bush is a plant that has or possesses the qualities, features or attributes “thorny”, “flowering” and “bushy”.

 

Further confusion arises in discussion because concepts, properties and qualities exist in two forms: as types and instances (values or facts).

The term "property" is used ambiguously to mean property type (“height in metres”) and/or property instance (1.74 metres).

The term "concept" is probably used more often for the property type than the property instance.

 

Beware also that the terms “class” and “type” are widely confused, for example, in programming languages.

We use “type” as above – with reference to the description of one individual thing (e.g. one planet).

We try to reserve class” to mean a set or group (e.g. all planets), which could have a property of its own (e.g. average volume).

The strict types used in mathematics and computing

The by-products of evolution now include mathematics and computing.

Mathematicians define mathematical objects in terms of strict types.

They form mathematical proofs using those types.

 

Computers read process types, perform process instances

They create data structures that instantiate data types.

They are based on boolean logic, in which everything is "true or false" (1 or 0).

 

Whether everything is ultimately describable in binary terms is a philosophical question worth pursuing.

But in practice, much computer input and output data is somewhere between true or false.

Fuzzy logic and artificial intelligence

The evolutionary story of description runs on to artificial intelligence.

What follows is edited from http://whatis.techtarget.com/definition/fuzzy-logic.

 

The words we use are not strict types (except within a very limited ontology).

Natural language (like most of the universe) is not reducible to true or false sentences.

A "state of matters" or "fact" that we recall or communicate has "degrees of truth".

 

0 and 1 may encode wholly false and wholly true; but there are degrees of truth in between.

E.g. the result of a comparing two things is not "tall" or "short" but ".38 of tallness."

 

Think of it this way

·       the strict types used in maths evolved from the fuzzy types in nature.

·       the binary or Boolean logic used in computers is a special case of the fuzzy logic in nature.

 

Fuzzy logic seems closer to the way our brains work.

We aggregate data and form a number of partial truths which we aggregate further into higher truths.

When certain thresholds are exceeded, further results such as motor reaction, are triggered.

 

A similar process is used in neural networks, expert systems and other artificial intelligence applications.

Fuzzy logic is essential to the development of human-like capabilities for AI (aka artificial general intelligence).

AI is the representation of generalized human cognitive abilities in software so that, faced with an unfamiliar task, the system can find a solution.

 

There are now robots that can perform the magic of abstracting a general type of thing from observations of particular things.

“Theoretical results in machine learning mainly deal with a type of inductive learning called supervised learning.

In supervised learning, an algorithm is given samples that are labeled in some useful way.

For example, the samples might be descriptions of mushrooms, and the labels could be whether or not the mushrooms are edible.

The algorithm takes these previously labeled samples and uses them to induce a classifier.

This classifier is a function that assigns labels to samples including the samples that have never been previously seen by the algorithm.

The goal of the supervised learning algorithm is to optimize some measure of performance such as minimizing the number of mistakes made on new samples.” Wikipedia

Conclusions and remarks

Biological and psychological evolution have developed and refined how we typify and describe reality.

The evolutionary story runs something like this.

·       The big bang and the space-time continuum

·       Sensing and reacting to continuous change

·       Sensing and reacting to discrete things

·       Remembering perceived entities and events

·       Creating and using mental models

·       Recognising family resemblances (fuzzy types)

·       Counting things

·       Abstracting logical mental models from physical ones

·       Verbalising mental models

·       Using words to label, define and communicate fuzzy types

·       The strict types used in mathematics and computing

·       Fuzzy logic and artificial intelligence

 

This story leads also us to a particular view of philosophy.

 

 

All free-to-read materials at http://avancier.website are paid for out of income from Avancier’s training courses and methods licences.

If you find the web site helpful, please spread the word and link to avancier.website in whichever social media you use.